Color requires our specifying three variables; to any color there corresponds a triplet of numbers, and we can think of any color as occupying a point in three-dimensional space. Top: Land's original formulation of the color-constancy problem seems to call for three kinds of cells, which compare the activation of a given set of cones (red, green, or blue) in one region of retina with the average activation of the same set in the surround. The result is three numbers, which specify the color at the region. Thus yellow, brown, dark gray, and olive green each has a corresponding triplet of numbers. We can therefore plot colors in a color space specified by three axes, for red, green, and blue. Bottom: A mathematically equivalent system also gives three numbers, and is probably closer to the way the brain specifies color. At any point on the retina, we can speak of red- greenness, the reading an instrument would give if it were to record the relative stimulation of red and green cones (zero for yellow or white). This value is determined for a particular region, and an average value is determined for the surround; then the ratio is taken. The process is repeated for yellow-blueness and black- whiteness. These three figures together are enough to specify any color. We can plot points in such a space in more than one way. The coordinate system can be Cartesian, with the three axes orthogonal and oriented in any direction or we can use polar or cylindrical coordinates. The Hering theory (and apparently the retina and brain) simply employ a different set of axes to plot the same space. This is doubtless an oversimplification because the blob cells making up the three classes are not like peas in pods but vary among themselves in the relative strengths of surrounds and centers, in their perfections in the balance between opponent colors, and in other characteristics, some still not understood. At the moment, we can only say that the physiology has a striking affinity with the psychophysics. You may ask why the brain should go to the trouble to plot color with these seemingly weird axes rather than with the more straightforward r, g, and b axes, the way the receptor layer of the retina does. Presumably, color vision was added in evolution to the colorless vision characteristic of lower mammals. For such animals, color space was one- dimensional, with all cone types (if the animal had more than one) pooled. When color vision evolved, two more axes were added to the one already present. It would make more sense to do that than to throw out the pooled system already present for black-white and then have to erect three new ones. When we adapt to the dark and are using only our rods, our vision becomes colorless and is again plotted along one axis, to which the rods evidently contribute. That would not be easy to do with r, g, and b axes.